A new study says that it takes longer for older drivers to notice pedestrians at night. And pedestrians are more visible to them if they wear reflective strips on their movable joints like such as shoulders, elbows, wrists, waist, knees and ankle. Eye doctors can test seniors to ensure that they are safe driving at night. Learn more.

Perspective Optometry (formerly Vision Source) Vancouver Optometrists Optometrist for kids and adults. Clinic Address: 7633 Nanaimo Street, Vancouver, B.C.; clinic telephone: 604-435-3931.
Showing posts with label driving. Show all posts
Showing posts with label driving. Show all posts
Sunday, June 22, 2014
How to be visible at night to older drivers
Labels:
biological motion,
driving,
older adults,
pedestrians,
safety,
seniors,
visibility
Saturday, August 31, 2013
Visual processing speed slows down as we age, making driving less safe - how to measure it and how to improve it
We slow down as we age. But can we get some of our youthful speed back, at least with respect to visual processing? Will treating visual processing weakness make older adults safer drivers? A new article in the September 2013 issue of the journal Vision Research suggests that the answers to these questions is "yes".
With respect to eyes and vision, the need for reading glasses is a common vision change that few manage to escape as they age. Another change, that is not as well known is changes in our visual processing speed.
Changes in visual processing speed affect all aspects of our daily life. Slower visual processing can make us generally inefficient as we are slow to take in and react to our visual world. It can also make us prone to accidents.
What can we do about declining visual processing speed? There is research that suggests that with the appropriate training, therapy and practice, visual processing speed can be improved for for many activities of daily living such as driving.
One way to test visual processing speed is a protocol developed by doctors of optometry called Useful Field of View (UFOV). It consists of a computer-generated series of three tests. The results are categorized into areas of visual processing weakness:
- rapid visual processing,
- selective visual processing, and
- divided attention.
The results of UFOV illustrate the difference between legal driving and safe driving. Legal driving requirements are based on visual acuity - how clearly you see at various distances and whether you have the necessary peripheral vision. Legal driving tests do not assess how useful your vision is to the task of driving. Safe driving is impacted by the driver's speed of visual information processing. The law has not caught up to vision science yet, but it is only a matter of time.
For people that have weak visual processing speed and who want to improve, developmental optometrists at our Vancouver eye clinic such as Dr. M.K. Randhawa, offer therapy programs to improve visual processing speed. Car accidents can radically change your life for the worse, anything that can be done to avoid them is welcome.
Related articles:
Optometrists identify seniors with high car-crash risk due to in-car distractions. Are Ford Sync and Dodge Uconnect dangerous?
Thursday, May 3, 2012
CITATION:
Vision Research
Visual Processing SpeedVision Res. 2013 Sep 20;90()52-56, C Owsley
map | directions | FREE parking | book an appointment
Labels:
driving,
elderly,
safety,
senior,
speed,
UFOV,
useful field of view,
visual processing
Wednesday, July 10, 2013
Dry eye and safe driving
Drivers need good vision to drive safely and legally. At our Vancouver eye clinic, our eye doctors diagnose and treat many eye and vision conditions that affect driving. But did you know that dry eye syndrome is one of them?
Dry Eye Syndrome (also called dry eye disease) has a significant impact on vision quality and can impair safe driving. Read more...
Related Articles
Severe dry eye syndrome after LASIK - See for Life
Feb 24, 2013
Dry eye syndrome after cataract surgery - See for Life
Dec 02, 2012
Dry eye syndrome hurts work productivity - See for Life
Oct 31, 2012
Depressed? Anxious? Maybe your eyes are too dry ... - See for Life
Apr 20, 2012
Caffeine improves dry eye syndrome - according to ... - See for Life
Apr 18, 2012
68% of women say that over-the-counter eye drops don't work for dry eye disease
Apr 28, 2012
Women, already the worst sufferers of dry eye, make it worse with makeup
Apr 06, 2012
Menopausal women are at high risk of developing dry eye disease
Apr 27, 2012
What can go wrong if you don't get treatment for your dry eye?
Nov 13, 2012
Does Restasis work?
Oct 30, 2012
Image courtesy of digitalart / FreeDigitalPhotos.net
Dry Eye Syndrome (also called dry eye disease) has a significant impact on vision quality and can impair safe driving. Read more...
Related Articles
Severe dry eye syndrome after LASIK - See for Life
Feb 24, 2013
Dry eye syndrome after cataract surgery - See for Life
Dec 02, 2012
Dry eye syndrome hurts work productivity - See for Life
Oct 31, 2012
Depressed? Anxious? Maybe your eyes are too dry ... - See for Life
Apr 20, 2012
Caffeine improves dry eye syndrome - according to ... - See for Life
Apr 18, 2012
68% of women say that over-the-counter eye drops don't work for dry eye disease
Apr 28, 2012
Women, already the worst sufferers of dry eye, make it worse with makeup
Apr 06, 2012
Menopausal women are at high risk of developing dry eye disease
Apr 27, 2012
What can go wrong if you don't get treatment for your dry eye?
Nov 13, 2012
Does Restasis work?
Oct 30, 2012
Image courtesy of digitalart / FreeDigitalPhotos.net
Labels:
driving,
dry eye,
dry eye disease,
dry eye syndrome,
safety
Thursday, May 3, 2012
Optometrists identify seniors with high car-crash risk due to in-car distractions. Are Ford Sync and Dodge Uconnect dangerous?
Drivers with Limited 'Useful Field of View' Should Avoid In-Car Distractions
A study in the April issue of Optometry and Vision Science, official journal of the American Academy of Optometry, has found that a computerized vision test can identify older drivers who are likely to have driving problems related to distractions in the car and who therefore have a high crash risk.The study authors, led by Joanne M. Wood, PhD, FAAO, of Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia, found that older drivers who show limitations on an optometric vision test called the "Useful Field of View" (UFOV) test make more driving errors when they are distracted. The finding provides a basis for making older drivers safer by predicting who will be more distractible on the road. Older drivers identified as distractible can benefit from minimizing distraction while driving - this makes driving safer for them and the drivers with whom they share the road.
Test Predicts Driving Problems Related to Distraction
The study looked at 92 drivers with an average age of 74 years who underwent the computerized UFOV test. Useful field of view is defined as "the area over which a person can extract information in a single glance without moving his or her head or eye." Drivers with limitations in UFOV are more likely to have problems in demanding driving situations, with an increased risk of crashes.
After the UFOV test, the drivers performed a closed-course driving test three times. On two of the three runs of the driving test, their car was fitted with visual or auditory distracters. The distracters were simple math problems presented on a video screen or audio speaker. The distractors mimic the degree of attention demanded by common in-car experiences such as conversations, navigating an ipod or smart phone, navigation system or in-car entertainment system.
Drivers who had limitations in UFOV were most likely to have problems on the driving test when there were visual or auditory distracters present in the car. These drivers also took longer to complete the driving test. This may be a reason for the slower driving commonly seen in older drivers.
The part of the UFOV test that assessed a driver’s "selective attention” was the best predictor of driving performance. Drivers with low selective attention scores had a very high crash risk. In contrast, older drivers who did better on the selective attention subtest had better overall performance on the driving test, even with distracters.
Previous research has shown that the UFOV test is highly effective in predicting crash risk among older adults, with or without vision problems. The new study suggests that distractibility is an important contributor to problems in driving performance and crash risk. That conclusion is consistent with recent research on the effects of increased distraction while driving, especially distractions from things like cell phone use.
Every year, cars come outfitted with more gadgets inside from GPS navigation, to touchscreens. My new Dodge Journey includes a car-driver interface (pictured above) that allows the driver to make phone calls by scrolling through an on-screen phone book downloaded from a cell phone and to browse an ipod music library on an dashboard integrated touch screen. These are all distractors and they are an example of how the driving environment is becoming more complex and distracting.
The interface can also be controlled by voice but that is also distracting because it involves paying close attention to audio instructions and prompts. This study’s results have important implications for the design of these kinds of in-vehicle devices and the safety of older drivers using them.
After the UFOV test, the drivers performed a closed-course driving test three times. On two of the three runs of the driving test, their car was fitted with visual or auditory distracters. The distracters were simple math problems presented on a video screen or audio speaker. The distractors mimic the degree of attention demanded by common in-car experiences such as conversations, navigating an ipod or smart phone, navigation system or in-car entertainment system.
Drivers who had limitations in UFOV were most likely to have problems on the driving test when there were visual or auditory distracters present in the car. These drivers also took longer to complete the driving test. This may be a reason for the slower driving commonly seen in older drivers.
The part of the UFOV test that assessed a driver’s "selective attention” was the best predictor of driving performance. Drivers with low selective attention scores had a very high crash risk. In contrast, older drivers who did better on the selective attention subtest had better overall performance on the driving test, even with distracters.
Older Drivers at Risk Should Minimize Distractions
Previous research has shown that the UFOV test is highly effective in predicting crash risk among older adults, with or without vision problems. The new study suggests that distractibility is an important contributor to problems in driving performance and crash risk. That conclusion is consistent with recent research on the effects of increased distraction while driving, especially distractions from things like cell phone use.
Every year, cars come outfitted with more gadgets inside from GPS navigation, to touchscreens. My new Dodge Journey includes a car-driver interface (pictured above) that allows the driver to make phone calls by scrolling through an on-screen phone book downloaded from a cell phone and to browse an ipod music library on an dashboard integrated touch screen. These are all distractors and they are an example of how the driving environment is becoming more complex and distracting.
The interface can also be controlled by voice but that is also distracting because it involves paying close attention to audio instructions and prompts. This study’s results have important implications for the design of these kinds of in-vehicle devices and the safety of older drivers using them.
Here is a funny TV commercial promoting the Dodge Journey's uconnect driver interface system. Will something like this be a dangerous distraction for older drivers?
Location: Vancouver, BC, Canada
2625 E 49th Ave, Vancouver, BC V5S 1J9, Canada
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)